The Impact of Poverty and Homelessness on Children’s Oral and Literate Language: Practical Implications for
Service Delivery
from a presentation at ASHA Schools Conference Milwaukee, Wisconsin
Save your time – order a paper!
Get your paper written from scratch within the tight deadline. Our service is a reliable solution to all your troubles. Place an order on any task and we will take care of it. You won’t have to worry about the quality and deadlines
Order Paper Now
July 28, 2012
By Celeste Roseberry-McKibbin, PhD
California State University, Sacramento
San Juan Unified School District
I. Key Points
● Statistics regarding poverty in the U.S. ● Factors that impact low-SES students’ linguistic and academic achievement ● Effects of poverty on oral and literate language development ● Suggestions for supporting low-SES parents in increasing their children’s language skills ● Strategies for professionals for increasing the oral and written language skills of low-SES students ● Executive functioning deficits in students and summary of remediation strategies
II. Understanding Variables Affecting Low-SES Students’ Performance
Background ● Never equate poverty with dysfunction. ● The term, poverty, often brings to mind the cultural differences that arise from race, ethnicity,
religion, country of origin, and ability or disability. However, in many countries, substantial cultural differences exist between people who are economically disadvantaged and those who are advantaged (Turnbull & Justice, 2012).
Variables • The standard of living for those in the bottom 10% is lower in the United States than in any other
developed nation, except the United Kingdom. • Poor families with three or more people spend about one third of their income on food. • Last year, 7.7% of African American women and 8.5% of Hispanic women worked in jobs that paid
at or below minimum wage, as compared to 4.3% of White men (www.nwlc.org, 2011). • African American and Hispanic women are more likely than white women to be heads of
households. • Married households’ median annual 2011 income was $71,830, while female-headed households
earned $32,597.
Effects of Homelessness • Homeless children and youth lack a fixed, regular, and adequate nighttime residence. • These children often live in cars, parks, public places, abandoned buildings, or bus or train
stations. • The cause is the inability of people to pay for housing; thus, homelessness is impacted by both
income and the affordability of available housing (National Alliance to End Homelessness, 2012). Potential Psychological and Physical Effects • Malnutrition • Illness • Hearing and vision problems • Housing problems (e.g., lead poisoning, homelessness, frequent moving, crowded conditions, no
place to play outside) • Neighborhood problems (e.g., violence) • Family stress • Fewer learning resources • Lack of cognitive and linguistic stimulation Observations ● When financial resources are stressed, there are higher rates of maternal depression. ● Compared with higher-income mothers, who tend to be more warm and verbal with their children,
low-income mothers often show lower levels of warmth, responsiveness, and sensitivity when interacting with young children. (Barrett & Turner, 2005; La Paro, Justice, Skibbe, & Plante, 2004; Neuman, 2009)
● The overall warmth and effect of a home, which promote caregiver-child bonding, form the very foundation of language development.
III. Definition of Situational vs. Generational Poverty (Payne, 2003; Roseberry-McKibbin, 2013)
Characteristics of Situational Poverty • Common for immigrants • Occurs for a shorter period of time • Usually the result of circumstances (divorce, illness, death) • People have a sense of pride and a belief in their ability to rise above their circumstances through
hard work. • They may refuse to accept offers of help as “charity.”
Characteristics of Generational Poverty • Affects a family for two generations or longer • Usually involves welfare • A common attitude is “I am stuck, and the world owes me.” • There is a short-term value system, which emphasizes survival in the present—not planning for
the future (e.g., long-range educational plans). Comparison of Situational and Generational Poverty The values of persons in situational and generational poverty may differ in a number of areas.
SITUATIONAL • Life priorities include achievement,
possessions, status.
• Money is to be saved, managed, invested.
• Religion is one of the accoutrements of life; fits into the person’s schedule.
• Time is to be valued; punctuality is critical; the future is important.
• Destiny is in our hands; we all have
choices; there is an internal locus of control.
• Education is crucial for getting ahead in
life, making good $$, being respected. • Entertainment is a reward for hard work;
money is used for education and life comforts; leftover $$ is used for entertainment after other priorities are met.
• Discipline is important; punishment/
negative consequences are about change; “don’t be sorry, be different.”
• Organization and planning are very important. Life is carefully scheduled into structured time slots. Structure is crucial: Calendars, iPhones, and other organizational devices proliferate.
• With language, formal register is used;
language is used to meet needs, get ahead in life.
• Interaction style values quiet;
conversational partners do not interrupt, but politely wait their turn.
GENERATIONAL • Survival, entertainment, relationships are
important; it’s all about the PRESENT. • Money is to be spent, especially on things
that bring pleasure in the moment. • Religion may be the center of much of life;
a great deal of time may be spent at the church.
• “You get there when you get there”; the
present is most important;
• “You can’t fight city hall”; there can be learned helplessness; there is an external locus of control.
• Education is valued in the abstract, not
emphasized as a real or attainable goal. • Entertainment plays a crucial role and is
highly valued; it may take precedence over education; the present is all we have (e.g., Why not enjoy life right now?); live in the moment.
• Punishment is not about change; it is
about penance and forgiveness; the person’s behavior continues as before.
• Organizational/planning devices are
virtually nonexistent. Clutter is common; structure is not valued. Planning ahead is not common; “living by the seat of your pants” is typical.
• Casual register is used; language is used
for entertainment and for survival. • There is constant background noise;
interruptions during conversation are common and expected.
Home Language • “Those shoes suck.” • “Gimme that apple.” • “Dude, that was totally stupid.”
School Language • “Those shoes are different than your
usual.” • “I’m hungry—that apple looks good.” • “Interesting idea—hadn’t thought of it that
way.”
IV. Factors Impacting Oral Language Language Characteristics Correlated With Low SES • Being poor does not cause children to have language and behavioral impairments. • Never equate poverty with dysfunction. • However, certain language and behavioral characteristics are associated with being from a low-
SES background (Nelson, 2010). Limited Access to Health Care • This issue can impact language skills. • If the mother is malnourished during pregnancy, the child’s brain development can be impacted. • Children who are often sick miss school. • If children are sick or hungry, they have difficulty learning; it is hard for them to concentrate. • Middle ear infections can impact listening and even written language (e.g., reading, spelling). Observations • There is a strong correlation between adults’ education and their income levels. • Long-term welfare dependency is associated with low literacy skills and lack of a high school
diploma. • In terms of educational level of caregivers, research has found that SES is more critical to a child’s
language development than ethnic background. • The factor most highly related to SES is the mother’s educational level. Caretakers Who Have Little Formal Education • They may not provide adequate oral language stimulation for children. • They may not believe that it is important to talk with babies and young children (who are not
treated as conversational partners).
Research Pruitt & Oetting (2009) • Children from low-income families have been shown to have limited input, in terms of volubility
and quality, when compared to children from wealthier families, and these differences have been linked to delayed language abilities.
Nelson (2010) • Children in low-income families are engaged more in talk about immediate daily living concerns—
for example, what to eat, wear, and do or not do. • Conversations in low-SES homes often do not extend beyond practical concerns. • One consequence of this is that low-SES children often have very concrete language. • They have difficulty understanding the abstract, decontextualized language of school. Hart & Risley (1995) • The researchers conducted longitudinal studies of families from various ethnic and SES
backgrounds. • Over several years, they observed behavior in the homes of 1- to 2-year-old children from three
groups: welfare, working class, and professional. • Hart and Risley concluded that SES made an “overwhelming difference” in how much talking went
on in a family. • The family factor most strongly associated with the amount of talking in the home was not
ethnicity, but SES. • In a 365-day year, children from professional families would have heard 4 million utterances.
• Children from welfare families would have heard 250,000 utterances. • The number of utterances in working class families fell somewhere in between. • Even by 3 years of age, the difference in vocabulary knowledge between children from welfare
homes and those from middle class homes was so great that—in order for the welfare children to gain a vocabulary equivalent to that of children from working class homes—the welfare children would need to attend a preschool program for 40 hours per week where they heard language at a level used in the homes of professional families..
IV. Strategies to Enhance Language Stimulation in Infants • Research shows that high-quality preschool programs portend the best short- and long-term
results for at-risk children from low-SES homes. • It is especially ideal if these programs can begin in infancy (The Carolina Abecedarian Project,
2006; Hart & Risley, 1995; Fowler, Ogston, Roberts-Fiati, & Swenson, 1995; Loeb, Fuller, Kagan, & Chang, 2004).
Strategies include the following: ● Read to babies (simple books with colorful pictures are best). ● Talk to the baby; face-to-face contact is ideal. ● Label common objects. ● Introduce music and singing ● Let the baby have a safe-glass mirror to look in. ● Point out and label body parts during activities such as dressing and bathing. ● Use short utterances with simple syntax. ● Heighten facial expressions, gestures, and intonation. ● Play turn-taking games such as pattycake and peek-a-boo. ● In very early infancy, introduce black-and-white objects. ● Imitate sounds the baby makes, and make new sounds. ● Make a habit of using greetings and leave takings (“Bye bye, Allison!”). ● Provide many opportunities for babies to put simple objects into containers and then take them
out. ● While doing household chores and errands, bring the baby along and describe what is happening. ● Introduce two languages from birth if possible.
Research Tamis-LeMonda (2001) • Caregivers’ responsiveness is a major key to language growth. • The extent to which mothers imitated their 13-month-old children predicted the timing of the
children’s later language milestones.
– For example, if the child said “Ba!” the mother said “Ball!” – This predicted the timing of things like the child’s development of her first 50 words and the
use of two-word combinations. • We can even imitate non-speech movements (e.g., smiling, yawning). • Mother’s rapid response correlated highly with the child’s vocalizations.
– The toddlers of high responders were 6 months ahead, language-wise, of toddlers of low responders; they were saying their first word at age 10 months and reaching other milestones by age 14 months.
– How often a mother initiated a conversation with her child was not predictive of language outcomes; the most significant factor was—if the child initiated—whether the mother responded.
– The most powerful mechanism for moving a baby from babbling to fluent speech was how a parent responded (or not) to a child’s vocalizations in the moment.
V. Strategies for Preschool Children
• Qi and Kaiser (2004) showed that some preschool children had emotional-behavioral issues that impacted their social interactions with other children.
• Kimochis can be used as a tool for supporting social skills (Dodge, Rice, & Grimm, 2010).
– Kimochis are plush toys that designate feelings. – They are accompanied by a program that teaches the seven keys to successful communication
(e.g., “Choose words that help instead of hurt.”).
To Increase Expressive Language Skills • It is important to talk with the child as much as possible. • Extensions are quite powerful:
– Child: “Kitty!” – Adult: “Yes, there is a black kitty sitting on the sidewalk.”
• We add new grammatic and semantic information to the child’s utterances, for example:
– Child: “I see bus!” – Adult: “Yes, look at that big yellow bus going down the road.”
● Working with parents as little as once a week can provide benefits (Roberts & Kaiser, 2011). • Extensions are very useful; they can be easily taught to parents, and they increase children’s
morpho-syntactic skills.
Research Lovelace & Stewart (2009) • Children from low-SES backgrounds are often limited in experiences needed to build background
knowledge for vocabulary growth because individual choices and experiences provided to these children overall are more limited than for groups with greater economic resources…Because experiences are limited, the potential for gaining word knowledge from a variety of opportunities is predictably reduced for these children. These early differences in children’s vocabulary knowledge have shown that even a small disadvantage grows into a larger one and becomes difficult to ameliorate without intervention.
• It is important to build low-SES children’s conceptual bases and then move into oral and literate language from there.
Justice (2010) and Roseberry-McKibbin (2007, 2013) • Phonological awareness is the ability to consciously reflect on and manipulate the sound system of
a language. • It is foundational to success in reading, writing, and spelling. • Low-SES preschoolers especially need to develop phonological awareness skills.
Koutsoftas, Harmon, & Gray (2009) • The researchers studied the effect of Tier 2 intervention for phonemic awareness in a response-to-
intervention (RtI) model in low-income preschool children. • Tier 1 instruction is high-quality, evidence-based classroom instruction. • Children who are not making adequate progress with this model are provided with Tier 2
intervention. Tier 2 intervention typically consists of short-term, high quality explicit instruction that is carried out in small groups by reading specialists, teachers, SLPs, and others.
• In this study, Tier 2 intervention for beginning sound awareness was provided twice a week (in 20-minute sessions) for 6 weeks by trained teachers and SLPs. The intervention was successful for 71% of the children.
• The authors concluded that the intervention was efficient and effective. Ukrainetz, Ross, & Harm (2009) • The researchers studied schedules of phonemic awareness treatment for kindergarteners. • In one schedule, the students were seen three times a week from September through December. • In another schedule, children were seen once a week from September through March. • There were large, maintained gains for children seen in both schedules. • It was found that the gains made from short, intense treatment were similar to those made from
continuous weekly treatment.
Justice, Kaderavek, Fan, Sofka, & Hunt (2009) • The researchers studied accelerating low-SES preschoolers’ literacy development through
classroom-based teacher-child storybook reading and explicit print-referencing. • They found that children whose teachers used print-referencing strategies showed larger gains on
three standardized measures of print knowledge than children whose teachers did not use the strategies.
• Examples of print referencing strategies used by teachers include the following:
– “This M in the red block is an uppercase letter. See how this uppercase letter is different than these lowercase letters?”
– “This word is the. This word is in this book all the time—can you help me find it?”
Gillam (2011) • Some low-SES (some bilingual) parents were given wordless books to read with their children;
“Looking for a Similar Assignment? Get Expert Help at an Amazing Discount!”